By David K. Shipler
Everybody you kill in the line of duty
becomes a slave in the afterlife.
--A white Los Angles
policeman, in a 1990s computer message.
Within
the array of stereotypes inflicted upon blacks in America over many
generations, the image of violence stands out. From slavery on, blacks have
been seen as dirty, ugly, stupid, immoral, alien, and dangerous. These fictions
become more or less prominent with time and circumstance, but they never quite
die away. Even when they are not translated into law or practice, they can lurk
as “implicit bias” that contaminates behavior. The label “dangerous” is
especially pernicious.
Much of
the brutal policing now being protested appears driven by the expectation that
blacks will be violent. That supposed trait appears regularly in surveys and
simulations. It is an old prejudice ingrained in American society, readily
activated by stress and triggering an officer’s split-second fear, which sometimes
leads to a shooting, but more often to warrantless frisks and auto searches,
handcuffing, and non-lethal physical force.
The
role of racial thinking is difficult to measure precisely. Thoughts and actions
do not inevitably coincide, and official statistics record end results, not causes.
During traffic stops producing no arrests over a thirteen-month period in
2013-14, for example, police in Oakland, CA handcuffed 1,466 African-Americans
but only 72 whites, Stanford psychologists reported.
While 72 percent of the department’s officers had handcuffed a black who wasn’t
arrested, 74 percent had never done so to a white. Handcuffing blacks was “a
script for what is supposed to happen,” the study concluded, a routine
presumably based on the violent stereotype but maintained as standard practice.
“Norms are a significant driver of behavior,” the psychologists observed. Other
experts have seen that rules issued from on high cannot readily overcome a
police department’s culture.
Where
the stereotype of violence exists, it can be reinforced by the slice of society
the police usually encounter—the criminal suspect, not the upstanding citizen.
That daily experience becomes a filter through which a world of hostility and
risk passes into perceptions, corrupting context and distorting reality. A 2019
study of 100 million traffic stops nationwide found blacks more
likely than whites to be stopped, but less so after dark when an officer couldn’t
see a driver’s race. Blacks who were pulled over were more likely than whites
to be searched. A 2016 examination
of files and mug shots determined that “the Whiter one appears, the more the
suspect will be protected from police force.”
Off-duty officers of color trying
to stop crimes are more at risk of being shot by fellow officers than their
white counterparts—comprising 10 of the 14 killed between 1995 and 2010,
according to a nationwide
study commissioned by the New York governor’s office. “Inherent or
unconscious racial bias plays a role in ‘shoot/don’t-shoot’ decisions made by
officers of all races and ethnicities,” the study declared.
The words, “all races and
ethnicities” deserve attention. When a group of psychologists examined
fatal shootings from 2015, they discovered that “black officers were just as
likely to shoot black citizens as white officers were,” said Joseph Cesario of
Michigan State University. The report put it another way: “As the proportion of
White officers in a fatal officer-involved shooting increased, a person fatally
shot was not more likely to be of a racial minority.” But it added a possible
reason: White victims were more prevalent because more whites than blacks were
mentally ill, and some whites were actually trying to get shot, committing
“suicide by cop.” Furthermore, black-on-black shootings rise when more black
officers are assigned to black neighborhoods, as they frequently are in big
cities.
A question is whether black
officers are as quick as some whites to shoot unarmed blacks, and whether they
share anti-black stereotypes. “People can have biases against their own
demographic groups,” said
Lorie Fridell, a criminologist at the University of South Florida. “Women can
have biases about women. Blacks can have biases about blacks.” But while
minority group members often internalize stereotypes held by the majority, how
those figure in the mix of motives for violence by black police is far from
clear. Class is probably a factor. Some poor inner city residents resent black
officers and let them know it. Some black officers return the contempt.
Others can reduce tensions. I saw
this while researching books on
civil liberties. When I spent nights reporting on an undercover narcotics unit
in Washington, D.C., I watched anger build among white cops as hostile black
residents hassled and berated them for arresting drug dealers. As tempers
reached a pitch, one black officer intervened skillfully to talk both sides
down. The unit called him “the reverend.”
The majority of police
officers are white males with less than a college degree, the cohort that voted
strongly for Donald Trump, who was endorsed by their union, the Fraternal Order
of Police. They generally hold views on race typical of the country’s white
conservatives, according to a 2016 survey
of 8,000 officers by the Pew Research Center.
White cops were nearly unanimous
(92 percent) in believing that the country had made the changes needed to guarantee
equal rights for blacks; only 6 percent thought that continued change was
needed. Their black colleagues said the opposite: only 29 percent saw adequate
change and 69 percent wanted more. A
similar divide existed on whether fatal encounters between blacks and police
were isolated incidents (72 percent of white officers, 43 of blacks) or part of
a broader problem (27 percent of whites, 57 percent of blacks).
Only 11 percent of law enforcement
academies overall require trainers to have four-year college degrees, according
to President Obama’s Task
Force on 21st Century Policing. The qualification is
required in just 7 percent of state police academies, 4 percent of academies
for municipal police, and none of those for sheriff’s deputies.
The militarization of police, both
in training and equipment, has been a major concern of reformers. Officers’
military backgrounds are correlated with the use of force. About 6 percent of
the country’s population has served in the military, but 19 percent of the
police. Indeed, policing is the third most popular job following active duty,
after driving trucks and the general category of managing.
Dallas officers who had been
deployed overseas with the military fired their weapons 2.9 times more than
cops with no military service, and those who hadn’t been deployed were 1.94
times as likely, the University of Texas School of Public Health reported
in 2018. In 2017, the Marshall Project found
that use-of-force complaints were more common against Boston and Miami police
who’d been in the military. In Albuquerque, one-third of the fatal police
shootings from January 2010 to April 2014 were by cops with military
backgrounds.
Attempts to quantify racial bias
using simulations have produced contradictory results. Joshua Correll, a
psychologist at the University of Colorado, has run tests
since 2000 with a video game showing images of black and white young men, some
holding guns, others cellphones or soda cans. Participants tended to shoot
armed blacks more often and sooner than they did armed whites, and to decide
more quickly to refrain from shooting unarmed men if they were white. In a
later study, police officers reacted most quickly, and correctly, to armed
blacks and unarmed whites—that is, they shot the blacks and did not shoot the
whites. Further simulations showed more racial bias in the shooting decisions
by officers of special units who had contact with minority gang members.
Training failed to diminish bias.
However, other studies have failed
to document a correlation between bias and action. Even when officers hold the
stereotype of blacks as violent, they don’t always behave accordingly, at least
in the simulations.
Two experiments in police
simulators recorded brain waves and physiological signs of a heightened threat
perception when black “suspects” were pictured. But neither that study nor a more
intricate simulation
found that racial bias translated into faster, more frequent shootings of blacks.
The simulation, in 2012-13 by
Washington State University, enlisted 80 officers in Spokane, 76 of whom were
white, who were paid to participate in uniform and with infrared guns that
looked like actual weapons—an attempt to get as close to reality as possible. The
cops took written and oral exams designed to document prejudice, including the
Harvard Implicit Association Test, which documents participants’ propensity to link
pictures of black and white faces with weapons. Ninety-six percent of the
officers registered implicit racial bias; 78 percent associated blacks with
weapons, none associated whites.
In four sessions with six scenarios
each, the officers were shown videos of actors playing people in similar
situations and clothing. The cops were more careful with blacks. They were three
times less likely to shoot unarmed blacks than unarmed whites. (The ratio was corrected
for the smaller number of black scenarios; in the raw numbers, 54 unarmed
whites and two unarmed blacks were shot.) The cops also took about two-tenths
of a second longer to decide to shoot armed blacks than armed whites, a delay
that researchers regarded as significant.
This was called “a counter bias” by
Lois James, one of the investigators. The report was ultimately titled “The
Reverse Racism Effect.” She postulated that the hesitation to shoot blacks grew
out of “people’s concerns about the social and legal consequences of shooting a
member of a historically oppressed racial group…paired with the awareness of
media backlash that follows an officer shooting a minority suspect.”
She might be right, but a
simulation is hardly reality. It contains no danger, no risk of death, no
adrenalin flowing, no actual fear.
In the Navy I attended a survival
school that concluded with being “captured,” crammed uncomfortably into a wooden
box, and “interrogated” with the threat of “torture.” It was the final lesson
of the last day of training, so I was extremely brave and recited only my name,
rank and serial number.
The simulation’s results do not
coincide with the Washington Post’s database
of fatal shootings by police, who have killed unarmed blacks since 2015 at a
rate two to three times the African-American representation in the country at
large. African-Americans constitute 13 percent of the population but between 25
and 40 percent of the unarmed citizens shot to death by police.
Black
|
All
|
Black
|
|
Unarmed
|
Unarmed
|
Share
|
|
2015
|
38
|
94
|
40.4%
|
2016
|
19
|
51
|
37.3%
|
2017
|
22
|
70
|
31.4%
|
2018
|
23
|
58
|
39.7%
|
2019
|
14
|
55
|
25.5%
|
2020
|
7
|
24
|
29.2%
|
The fitful start of a tentative downward trend is visible, both in absolute numbers and percentages. But it’s too early for optimism. The table above shows deaths recorded to June 11, 2020, and this year’s numbers are already on track to match or exceed last year’s. Furthermore, they do not include deaths that don’t involve firearms, hence George Floyd is not in the figures. Nor is Freddie Gray, who died in a Baltimore police van in 2015 from injuries to his spinal cord. How many others have escaped the database is unknown.
What is known is the resilience of
racial stereotyping, conscious and unconscious, which corrupts society until,
as in the civil rights movement, it mobilizes enough Americans to act.
"The vast majority of police officers are white males with less than a college degree" -- I don't think that's probably true. As of 2016 male White non-Hispanic officers were 64% of the total. Then, roughly 1/3 of police have 4-year degrees, so that drops to about 43%. So most likely a plurality of police are non-degreed White males, but certainly not a "vast majority."
ReplyDeleteThank you. Good catch. I've deleted "vast," but by my calculations there is a likely majority in that demographic. I wouldn't automatically subtract white Latinos from the total of white males, given both their rising support for Trump (32% voted for him) and their surveyed attitudes on race. Here are the 2016 figures: White non-Hispanic males: 64.4%, Hispanic males: 10.4%, non-college degrees: 70%.
ReplyDelete